written by Bettina Salmelin
A
study of the difference of the importance of early stimulation in different
breeds of dogs
Technical Summary
As the
number of purebred dogs (Canis familiaris) is increasing all over the
world and the importance and status of dogs is increasing, the importance of
breeding mentally stable dogs is becoming more important (FCI, 2008). The
amount of canines used as an aid for disabled has increased enormously in
the few past decades (The Seeing Eye, 2008).
The
majority of the behavior of the dog is reflected from its experiences from
its first seven weeks of life (Scott & Marston, 1950). Early stimulation and
the possibility of triggering and activating specific signals at early
stages of the lifecycle stimulates the brain before it would normally be
stimulated. The advantage of this is that the puppies will become more
resistant to stressful situations, more stable emotionally and with a higher
learning capacity (Fox 1978).
The
problems of early stimulation are that the limit of stimulation that
produces favourable results is still unclear. If the brain is over
stimulated at a very early stage, the brain does not learn to relax and the
puppy will constantly feel stressed and unsafe (Scott & Marston, 1950).
Lay summary
Purebred dogs (Canis familiaris) are becoming increasingly popular in
the world. There are over 400 different breeds recognized by the Federation
Cynologique Internationale (FCI, 2008). People have realized that dogs are
of great economical benefit to humans; they can detect mines, smell drugs,
find lost people and will be always be loyal to the owner.
With
the dog population increasing rapidly, the importance of mentally stable
dogs is of greater importance than before. The use of dogs as aids for
disabled people is also increasing, and the mental and emotional stability
of these guide and help dogs is of grave importance
(The Seeing Eye, 2008).
The
majority of the behavior of the dog is reflected from its experiences from
its first seven weeks of life (Scott & Marston, 1950). It is important the
breeders know the best way they can help to breed mentally stable dogs.
Background
The development of the canine (Canis familiaris)
behavioural patterns can be divided into four distinct learning phases; the
prenatal-, neonatal-, transition-, and the socialization- & juvenile phases.
Arguably the most important phase is the socialization phase, but the others
are also very important.
The prenatal period is usually much overlooked, since it is
very hard to determine the effects of external stimuli that appear long
after the stimulation. However, there have been studies in rodents which
conclude that if the parent has been stressed during pregnancy, the puppies
will become more reactive to emotions and external stimuli (Thompson 1957;
Thompson, Watson & Charlesworth, 1962).
The neonatal phase occurs during the first two weeks of life
and the puppy is still unresponsive to environmental stimuli, blind and
deaf. Even though the puppy is still unresponsive, stress during this time
adapts its the pituitary-adrenocortical system to react better later on in
stressful situation (Levine 1967). The benefits of this adaption are
improved stress resistance, emotional stability and increased learning
capacity (Fox 1978).
Battaglia (2001) showed similar results after handling
puppies each day 25 seconds between 3-16 days after birth. The results
showed that the littermates that had been stimulated for this period of time
had improved cardio vascular performance (heart rate), stronger heart beats,
stronger adrenal glands, were more tolerance to stress and had greater
resistance to disease. They were also more active and dominant over those
which were not stimulated. In nature, this would lead that the stimulated
puppies would be evolutionally more beneficial in surviving.
Figure 5: Tactical
stimulation (between toes), head held erect, head pointed down, supine
position and thermal stimulation (Battaglia, 2001).
C. Sighieri
et al.
(2006) conducted the same experiments as Battaglia
(2001) had done, handling the puppies between 3 to 19 days after birth. The
results they gathered at the age of 2 months by seeing how they respond in
stressful situations and how active they were. The results showed that the
most significant differences that arose were from the Isolation test, where
the stimulated group, in average, yelped later and spent more time exploring
the new room. There was no significant difference in the Arena test, where
the puppies were left in a room with a person to see how actively the
puppies played with the new objects.
Figure 1:
A puppy can feel safe and sound in human hands if properly introduced.
Newfoundland puppy in supine position (Salmelin, 2008).
Transition period occurs during 13-20 days after birth, when
the puppies change to act more like adults. They start to see and hear and
move around. As for learning and effects of the early stimulation, this
phase resembles the neonatal period. They do not have the capacity to
remember and learn very fast at this stage yet. They show only slight
increases in ability of performance and rate of learning, but it has been
shown that this phase is not yet the most important one, when considering
the effect to the later stages of life (Scott & Fuller, 1965).
The fourth phase contains the socialization and the juvenile
periods. They are very closely related and overlap to some extent, so they
are usually clumped together. During primary socialization the puppies form
direct relationships with their littermates, pack members and parents. They
also for tight connections to the environment they are reared in (Scott &
Fuller, 1965).
Figure 2 & 3: Stimulated puppies form tight relationships with the
littermates, parents and humans (Salmelin, 2005).
Freedman et al. (1961) was the first to conclude from their
experiments that the largest part of socialization of dogs occurs between
2-13 weeks. The results from their experiment, handling different groups of
puppies intensely for a week between 0-14 weeks, showed that the puppies
handled between 5 weeks and 9 weeks were most socialized.
Scott & Fuller (1965) specified that by the age of 8 weeks, the puppies
should have been socialized to everyone they will meet and form a bond in
their lives, by 12 weeks they should have met everything they will encounter
in later on. The introduction to new things after that window will result in
inadequate socialization, due to the fearfulness and insecurity of the
puppy. If the window has been missed, it is still possible to socialize them
between 6-7 months, but it will take longer (Woolpy & Ginsberg 1967; Nieburh
et al., 1980).
However, in a recent study the period during which the
puppies should leave has been criticized. The littermates taken out at 6
weeks had a higher mortality rate, were more stressed and more susceptible
to disease than the littermates kept until 12 weeks. They did, however, show
the same level of socialization. (Slabbert & Rasa, 1993).
As many studies above have concluded; the childhood of a
puppy is the most important factor in the formation of the character and
degree of socialization in dogs
(Scott & Marston, 1950).
If the window is missed, it is very hard to obtain good
results (Scott
& Fuller, 1965).
Therefore it would be wise to conduct a study which
determines more accurately what should be done, what effect it has, and
which exercises are more important in other breeds compared to others.
Figure 4: A calm Newfoundland puppy under stimulation (Salmelin, 2005).
The “childhood” of a dog (Canis familiaris) is the
most important factor in the formation of the character and degree of
socialization in dogs
(Scott & Marston, 1950).
If the window is missed, it is very hard to obtain good
results (Scott
& Fuller, 1965).
There
is evidence that the transition period in wolves starts slightly earlier,
and their development is faster compared to that of dogs (Frank & Frank
1982, 1985; Zimen, 1987). Can
this difference be seen within the same species?
Reference list
Battaglia, C. L. (2001) Developing Higher Achievers. [online]
http://www.breedingbetterdogs.com/early.pdf
[Accessed 12th March 2008].
FCI, (2008)
Federation Cynologique Internationale. [online]
http://www.fci.be
[Accessed 12th March 2008].
Fox, M. W. (1978). The Dog: Its
Domestication and Behavior. New York: Garland STMP Press.
Frank, H. & Frank, M. G. (1982).
On the effects of domestication on
canine social development and behavior. Applied Animal Ethology, 8,
507-25.
Frank, H. & Frank, M. G. (1985). Comparative
manipulation-test performance it ten-week-old wolves (Canis lupus)
and Alaskan malamutes (Canis familiaris): a Piagetian interpretation.
Journal of Comparative Psychology,
99,
266-74.
Freedman, D. J., King, J. A., Elliot, O. (1961). Critical
periods in the social development of dogs. Science,
133,
1016-17.
Levine, S. (1967). Maternal and environmental influences on
the adrenocortical response to stress in weanling rats. Science,
156,
258-60.
Nieburh, B. R., Levinson, M., Nobbe, D. E. & Tiller, J. E.
(1980). Treatment of an incompletely socialized dog. In Canine Behavior,
ed. B. L. Hart, p. 83. Santa Barbara, CA: Veterinary Practice Publishing Co.
Salmelin, B. R. M., (2005) Larinkallion H-litter of
Newfoundlands.
Scott,
J. P. & Fuller, J. L. (1965).
Genetics and the social behavior of
the dog. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Scott,
J.P., Marston, M. V. (1950). Critical periods affecting the development of
normal and mal-adjustive social behavior of puppies. Journal of Genetic
Psychology, 77, 25-60.
Slabbert, J. M. & Rasa, O. A. E. (1993). The effect of early separation from
the mother on pups in bonding to humans and pup health. Journal of the
South African Veterinary Association, 64, 4-8.
Sighieri, C., Mariti, C., Martelli, F., Baragli, P., Ducci, M. & Gazzano, A.
(2006). Effects of postnatal
handling on the ontogenesis of canine behaviour. Veterinary Research
Communications. 30 (Suppl. 1), 211-13.
The Seeing Eye, (2008) [online]
http://www.seeingeye.org/
[Accessed 12/03/08]
Thompson, W. R. (1957). Influence of prenatal and maternal anxiety on
emotionality in young rats. Science, 125, 698-9.
Thompson, W. R., Watson, J. & Charlesworth, W. R. (1962). The effects of
prenatal maternal stress on offspring behavior in rats. Psychological
Monographs, 76, 1-26.
Woolpy, J. H. & Ginsberg, B. E. (1967). Wolf socialization; A study of
temperament in a wild social species. Amercan Zoologist, 7,
357-63.
Zimen, E. (1987). Ontogeny of
approach and flight behavior towards humans in wolves, poodles and
wolf-poodle hybrids. In Man and Wolf, ed. H. Frank, pp. 275-92.
Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Dr W. Junk Publishers.
|